The Conditioning Effect on Celebrity Multiple Endorsements

This research adapts experimental design to investigate the effect of conditioning or not and pre-exposure or not on brand attitude, so it is a 2×2=4 factorial design. The results show that the brand attitude of conditioning group is significantly higher than that of unconditioning group. The brand attitude with pre-exposure is significantly higher than that without pre-exposure. Conditioning or not and pre-exposure or not have significant interaction. No matter the celebrity is pre-exposure or not, the brand attitude is higher under conditioning process.

Authors:



References:
[1] M. A. Kamins and K. Gupta, "Congruence between spokesperson and
product type: A matchup hypothesis perspective," Psychology &
Marketing, vol. 11, no. 6, pp. 569-586, 1994.
[2] C. Tripp, T. D. Jensen, and L. Carlson, "The effects of multiple product
endorsements by celebrities on consumers- attitudes and intentions,"
Journal of Consumer Research, vol. 20, pp. 535-54, 1994.
[3] K. L. Keller, Strategic Brand Management, 2th ed., Prentice-Hall Inc,
2008.
[4] N.-H. Um, "Exploring the effects of single vs. multiple products and
multiple celebrity endorsements," Journal of Management and Social
Sciences, vol. 4, no. 2, pp. 104-114, 2008.
[5] G. McCracken, "Who is the celebrity endorser? Cultural foundations of
the endorsement process," Journal of Consumer Research, vol. 16, no. 3,
pp. 310-321, 1989.
[6] H. H. Friedman and L. W. Friedman, "Endorser effectiveness by product
type," Journal of Advertising Research, vol. 19, no. 5, pp. 63-71, 1979.
[7] J. B. Freiden, "Advertising spokesperson effects: An examination of
endorser type and gender on two audiences," Journal of Advertising
Research, vol. 24, pp. 33-41, 1984.
[8] J. C. Mowen and S. W. Brown, "On explaining and predicting the
effectiveness of celebrity endorser," Advertising in Consumer Research,
vol. 8, pp. 437-441, 1980.
[9] F. Heider, The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations, New York:Wiley,
1958.
[10] G. Dyer, Advertising as Communication, London and New York:
Routledge, 1982.
[11] B. Z. Erdogan and P. Kitchen, "Getting the best out of celebrity
endorsers," Admap, pp. 17-20, Apr. 1998.
[12] A. Behr and A. Beeler-Norrholm, "Fame, fortune, and the occasional
branding misstep: When good celebrity go bad," Intellectual Property
&Technology Law Journal, vol. 18, no. 11, pp. 6-13, 2006.
[13] I. P. Pavlov, Conditioned reflexes, London: Clarendon Press, 1927.
[14] L. G. Schiffman and L. L. Kanuk, Consumer Behavior, 7th Ed., New
Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc, 2000.
[15] W. F. Bruewer, "There is no convincing evidence for operant or classical
conditioning in adult humans," in W. B. Weimer, and D. S. Palermo
(Ed.), Cognition and the Symbolic Processes, 1974, pp. 1-42.
[16] G. J. Gorn, "The effects of music in advertising on choice behavior: A
classical conditioning approach," Journal of Marketing, vol. 46, no. 1, pp.
94-101, 1982.
[17] C. T. Allen and T. J. Madden, "A closer look at classical conditioning,"
Journal of Consumer Research, vol. 12, no. 12, pp. 301-315, 1985.
[18] T. A. Shimp, E. W. Stuart, and R. W. Engle, "A program of classical
conditioning experiments testing variations in the conditioned stimulus
and context," Journal of Consumer Research, vol. 18, pp. 1-12, 1991.
[19] M. E. Blair and T. A. Shimp, "Consequences of an unpleasant experience
with music: Second-order negative conditioning perspective," Journal of
Advertising, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 35-41, 1992.
[20] B. D. Till, S. M. Stanley, and R. Priluck, "Classical conditioning and
celebrity endorsers: An examination of belongingness and resistance to
extinction," Psychology & Marketing, vol. 25, no. 2, 1pp. 79-196, 2008.
[21] J. F. Hair Jr., R. E. Anderson, R. L. Tatham, and W. C. Black,
Multivariate Data Analysis, 5th ed., Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall,
1998.